one case, children were asked to put their names and addresses down before getting candy from the bowl—in another case they were not asked for their names. Then, in both cases, the experimenter left the room—but not before telling the children to only take one candy from the bowl. Predictably, the children were more likely to take extra candy if they were anonymous. For a more recent example, look no further than online videogame communities. A study of online gaming communities conducted in 2015 offered these conclusions: that cheating is much more frequent when people are playing with strangers—and that cheating in online games can be solved by identification of group members. WE REFUSE TO TAKE PERSONAL RESPONSIBILITY This sounds like a terrible mission statement, but it’s a common human reflex to take the blame off ourselves when we're in groups—this tendency is known as the “diffusion of responsibility.” In some cases, there is ambiguity about who is responsible, so this assumption is justified—yet diffusion occurs even when responsibility is not impacted by the presence of others. The previously mentioned experiment on trick-or-treaters had a few conditions that looked at this: children in groups were more likely to take extra candy than children that were alone; and when one child was identified and held responsible for the actions of the group, children were less likely to take candy; but children were even less likely when they were all identified and told they would be held responsible. A HOMEGROWN MOB: THE 2011 VANCOUVER STANLEY CUP RIOT This example is one we all probably already know about, and some of us might have even personally witnessed. The hockey riot made international headlines for how violent and chaotic it got. Many of the concepts we have already discussed were present at this infamous 2011 event; at the end of the game, a few fans threw bottles at the screen and then many copied this action. Soon enough a horde of people were throwing bottles, and that intense group emotion was quickly transferred to the destruction of police cars. Another interesting human behaviour that was at work during these riots is something referred to as emotional contagion: when individuals “catch” a mob’s intense emotion and inevitably get swept up by it. Studies (like Emotional contagion in organizational life) have shown that people can feel the emotions they are imitating subconsciously through the natural process of mimicry. This does not fare well in riot situations—people are easily influenced by the passion of the crowd, so they act according to this overflow of emotion. While this emotion is commonly assumed to be anger or desperation, this Vancouver riot was fueled by a different emotion: excitement. In an article for the Vancouver Sun, UBC sociologist Rima Wilkes said that the rioters “weren't even angry.” She goes onto say that “they were having fun.” Emotional contagion like this is also qualified by the fact that it can be a trance-like state. As Douglas Psychology Professor James Barton pointed out in an interview with the Other Press, Vancouver rioters said they could not believe their own actions. This certainly seems like another long list of things to worry about, but studies have shown that being aware of subconscious processes helps in the prevention of making mistakes because of said processes. When asked about helpful preventative measures for herd mentality, Douglas Professor James Barton emphasized on the importance of education. Understanding that it “only takes a certain amount of the population that is intent on having other people conform” to achieve that goal is essential to remember. It doesn’t take much to sway people to conform—but it also doesn’t take much for individuals to educate themselves so they can avoid falling into recognizable traps. CC YouTube commenters are jerks, and science can explain why. Illustration by Athena Little